Skip to main content
  • Methodological intersections
  • Open access
  • Published:

Critical design choices in healthcare simulation education: a 4C/ID perspective on design that leads to transfer

Abstract

Background

Healthcare simulation education often aims to promote transfer of learning: the application of knowledge, skills, and attitudes acquired during simulations to new situations in the workplace. Although achieving transfer is challenging, existing theories and models can provide guidance.

Recommendations

This paper provides five general recommendations to design simulations that foster transfer: (1) emphasize whole-task practice, (2) consider a cognitive task analysis, (3) embed simulations within more comprehensive programs, (4) strategically combine and align simulation formats, and (5) optimize cognitive load. We illustrate the application of these five recommendations with a blueprint for an educational program focusing on simulation activities.

Conclusions

More evidence-informed approaches to healthcare simulation might require a paradigm shift. We must accept that a limited number of simulations is not enough to develop complex skills. It requires comprehensive programs that combine simulation sessions with workplace learning.

Background

Simulation-based education (SBE) in the health professions uses immersive techniques to interactively replicate substantial aspects of the real world [20]. In most instances, SBE provides learners with opportunities to acquire a set of competencies, ultimately leading to the transfer of learning, that is, the application of said competencies in new situations in the workplace. However, even experienced instructional designers find this aspect challenging. Successful transfer requires attention to several key steps: (a) a thorough analysis of content, learners, and the context,Ā (b) the design of a coherent blueprint based on solid instructional theories; (c) the development of high-quality teaching and learning activities and materials; (d) the implementation at many levels in the organization; and (e) a critical evaluation of all of the above (i.e., the ADDIE approach, see [47]).

Instructional theories provide helpful guidelines to design effective instruction. Examples include the Four-Component Instructional Design model (4C/ID; [57]) and the First Principles of Instruction [37]. In this paper, we present five recommendations for designing programs to promote learning transfer. We illustrate these recommendations using a worked example of a 4C/ID-based blueprint using the evaluation and management of patients with respiratory distress as a complex skill. Finally, we discuss some general implications.

Transfer of learning

According to 4C/ID, two main processes promote transfer of learning. Strongly simplified, we can label them as variation and repetition. Variation refers to variability of practice [32, 56]. In clinical practice, each set of circumstances is unique and specific to the context. Patients, diagnoses, and interventions are different. By replicating this variability in educational programs, we train learners to apply their knowledge flexibly and adapt their approach to specific tasks at hand. Nonrecurrent aspects of complex skills are performed differently in different professional tasks, such as diagnosing, clinical reasoning, dealing with complications, or communication. These skills often involve flexible problem-solving, decision-making, or reasoning appropriate to the situation at hand. To perform them successfully, learners require a broad and highly organized knowledge base that enables them to generate potential solutions. To develop this knowledge base, or construct cognitive schemas, learners must engage with varying learning tasks that stimulate them to abstract away from these concrete experiences or examples (i.e., inductive learning). Schema construction is also promoted by studying supportive materials and linking new information to existing knowledge (i.e., elaboration). Learners then transfer their learning by applying the rich and integrated schemas developed during training to improve their performance in novel tasks and situations. The richer and more integrated the schemas, the more likely that transfer occurs.

Whereas variation is essential for nonrecurrent aspects, repetition is essential for recurrent aspects. Recurrent aspects are performed identically in different variations of professional tasks (e.g., performing a physical examination or using a surgical instrument). Because they are identical, they can be trained repeatedly until their performance becomes quick, efficient, and sometimes even automated. The primary learning process here is schema automation, stimulated through specific how-to instruction combined with repetitive practice of recurrent aspects. Repeated practice links specific situations to specific actions by creating cognitive IFā€“THEN rules in memory (i.e., rule formation). The more often these rules are practiced, the quicker and easier they will be activated in the future (i.e., strengthening). Here, transfer of learning manifests when cognitive rules developed during training improve the accuracy and speed of the same aspects in a new task. The more often the recurrent aspect is practiced, the stronger and more refined the rules become. Thus, transfer becomes more likely.

These two interrelated transfer mechanisms work in parallel, as shown in Fig.Ā 1 [55]. Developing rich cognitive schemas leads to a broad knowledge base that enables learners to reflect on their performance and learn from their successes and failures, extending and refining their cognitive schemas in the process. Practice and reflection also lead to the automation of schemas that make the performance of recurrent aspects fast and effortless, reducing mistakes and freeing up cognitive resources. Learners can then better apply these resources to nonrecurrent skills requiring reasoning and problem-solving, completing the cycle.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The relationship between the two transfer mechanisms

In summary, achieving transfer relies on two parallel mechanisms: (a) driving schema construction to allow learners to perform unfamiliar task aspects in new situations, which requires variation, and (b) fostering schema automation to enable learners to effortlessly perform familiar aspects of new tasks, which requires repetition [26].

We now provide five recommendations for designing healthcare simulation education that fosters transfer of learning. The recommendations are a mix of theory-informed prescriptions stemming from 4C/ID and practical guidelines for applying them. Note that there are no quick strategies that clinicians can apply in their design and teaching tomorrow. Instead, these recommendations offer a first step toward better frameworks and a deeper fundamental understanding of making instructional design decisions.

Recommendation 1: Emphasize whole-task practice

Consider the complex skill of assessing and managing pediatric patients with respiratory distress who present to the emergency department. These situations require clinicians to coordinate several component or ā€œconstituentā€ skills, such as rapidly assessing the patientā€™s appearance, performing a focused history and physical examination, considering triggering factors, initiating appropriate therapy, communicating with caregivers, and collaborating with other medical staffā€”all while dealing with a potentially stressful environment. In a whole-task approach, learning is centered on realistic tasks that require learners to practice these different aspects in realistic relation to each other. This approach promotes complex learning, in which learners integrate knowledge, skills, and attitudes and coordinate required constituent skills as they would during real-life clinical tasks. In other words, they practice the nonrecurrent aspects (e.g., clinical reasoning) and the recurrent aspects (e.g., clinical examinations) in the same task, and sometimes even simultaneously (i.e., communicating with caregivers while performing a physical exam).

By focusing on authentic professional tasks in simulation activities, educators avoid compartmentalization, which occurs when learners work on knowledge, skills, and attitudes separately. Unfortunately, compartmentalization is widespread: knowledge is addressed in lectures, skills are trained in the skills lab, and attitudes are developed with role-plays. As a result, learners lack opportunities to integrate all three domains as they must in clinical practice, simultaneously applying their knowledge while performing skills and demonstrating attitudes. Whole-task practice also avoids fragmentation, or the disconnected training of isolated constituent skills (i.e., part-task training). This pitfall is obvious when ā€œtechnicalā€ and ā€œnon-technicalā€ skills are trained separately, even though they are performed concurrently in real-life tasks (e.g., [5, 9]). Training parts of a task while neglecting others creates problems because whole tasks represent more than the sum of their parts. Research demonstrates that whole-task training is more effective than part-task training for fostering transfer of learning to new situations [30, 62].

The 4C/ID model recommends using increasingly complex whole tasks as the backbone of learning programs to develop these integrated competencies. Part-task training then complements whole-task training instead of the other way around, a typical sequence in traditional curricula. This way, learners first confront whole tasks, thus learning why they must train specific constituent skills in isolation. Educational programs promote transfer of learning when they address the necessary coordination and integration by alternating between whole-task practice and part-task practice as complexity increases [16, 18, 19]. The workplace represents an ideal place to ā€œzoom outā€ to the whole task. For example, surgical trainees receive supervised whole-task practice in the operating room, then ā€œzoom inā€ and practice part-tasks on a box trainer, and then ā€œzoom outā€ to integrate these constituent skills in the operating room. Thus, part-task training complements whole-task training. Designers should consider this integration of part-task and whole-task training and avoid presenting part-task practice as isolated events.

Recommendation 2: Consider a cognitive task analysis

Educators frequently conduct needs assessments when developing simulation programs to reveal gaps between actual and desired performance. They then formulate lists of learning outcomes to fill those gaps and select instructional methods to reach each outcome [44]. For example, cognitive learning outcomes (e.g., clinical reasoning) are addressed with virtual patients [12], affective learning outcomes (e.g., empathic communication) are taught with human simulation [39], and psychomotor skills (e.g., operating ultrasound devices) are taught with part-task simulations [38].

While needs assessments reveal essential learning outcomes, they may encourage part-task approaches that focus only on targeted outcomes while ignoring other aspects of the whole task (i.e., risking compartmentalization and fragmentation). Thus, a needs assessment may not provide all the necessary information about which nonrecurrent and recurrent skills learners must coordinate in a whole task. In addition, evidence shows that clinical experts who teach recall only about 30% of their automated decisions and strategies, leading them to fill these memory gaps with perhaps faulty assumptions when explaining procedures to learners [10]. A more reliable approach is cognitive task analysis (CTA). Many types of CTA exist, but the general approach involves document study, observations, and detailed interviews with professional task performers, subject-matter experts, and expert teachers to deconstruct professional tasks into their constituent skills. This deconstruction results in a skill hierarchy that visualizes the constituent skills necessary to perform the complex skill (see Fig.Ā 2). Further knowledge elicitation techniques uncover the required cognitive strategies (i.e., systematic approaches to carry out the task) and the domain knowledge needed for the performance of nonrecurrent skills, and the cognitive IFā€“THEN rules needed for the performance of recurrent skills. A thorough description of CTA is outside the scope of this paper, but Clark et al. [11], Van MerriĆ«nboer and Kirschner [59], and Tjiam et al. [53] offer good starting points.

Fig. 2
figure 2

A condensed skill hierarchy for ā€œassessing and managing pediatric patients with respiratory distress in the emergency department.ā€ NR, nonrecurrent; R, recurrent; TBA, to-be-automated

A CTA and accompanying skill hierarchy enable the design of learning tasks that require the coordination of logical combinations of constituent skills, just like real-life tasks. Designers can also describe and classify the desired exit behavior for each constituent skill. Nonrecurrent skills require cognitive schemas for reasoning and decision-making because they are different each time, demanding variation. Recurrent skills that are similar across tasks and for which cognitive rules can be formed by the end of the training are labeled recurrent, requiring repetition. Critical recurrent skills that must be automated by the end of the training are labeled to-be-automated recurrent. This classification process allows designers to design whole-task practice with the required variation and repetition for the respective aspects.

Recommendation 3: Embed SBE in more comprehensive programs

We do not recommend basing an educational program entirely and solely on simulations (i.e., SBE). Instead, a thorough needs analysis and CTA provide primary inputs for the content, structure, and media use, meaning that a well-designed program often includes whole-task practice in both simulated and workplace settings. Educators often overestimate the contribution of single simulation sessions in terms of deep learning or transfer of learning, even when they include high-quality debriefing afterward. A short series of simulations provide insufficient practice and variation in task characteristics necessary to prepare learners for the variability of clinical work. Competency development takes time! Accordingly, designers should not view isolated simulations as a complete training program but as a group of ingredients that should be mixed with other ingredients to create a sophisticated dish.

According to 4C/ID, a complete training program includes four components. The programā€™s backbone consists of whole learning tasks (component 1). Many learning tasks can be simulations, from paper-based case studies to role-plays, simulated patient encounters, or immersive training with simulation mannequins, but also real-life professional tasks. Supportive information (component 2) helps learners with nonrecurrent aspects. This information can be presented in lectures, workshops, demonstrations, observations, readings, podcasts, e-learning modules, and AR or VR content. Learners study these materials before or during whole-task training. Procedural information (component 3) is presented just-in-time during the learning tasks to help with recurrent aspects. Instructors can provide corrective feedback or materials with how-to instructions, such as job aids, reference cards, manuals, or checklists. Finally, for to-be-automated skills, repeated practice on part-task simulators (component 4), such as box-trainers, can be employed to reach accurate and fast performance.

These four components simultaneously stimulate schema construction and automation, thereby increasing the chances for transfer. In such programs, simulations do not stand alone but are combined with other learning activities to optimally support competency development. Following this line of reasoning, simulation-enhanced education would be a more appropriate term than simulation-based education. Moreover, transfer is much more likely when combining simulations with workplace tasks, such as observations, debriefing participation, or guided practice with support from peers or supervisors [21, 43]. This combination of workplace learning and simulation promotes transfer learning from simulations to the workplace and helps learners bring relevant work experiences to simulations.

Recommendation 4: Combine and align different simulation activities

Specific simulation activities might be effective for whole-task practice, others for working on nonrecurrent aspects, and yet others for recurrent aspects and aspects that must be automated. Therefore, designers should combine simulation activities so learners may optimally develop, practice, and refine their competencies. These complicated design decisions involve balancing key considerations:

  • Effectiveness: What is the best way to attain the competencies necessary for whole task performance?

  • Efficiency: What does this activity cost in terms of resources such as development time, budget, and personnel?

  • Appeal: Will my learners and teachers enjoy this activity?

Thus, educators should not ask, ā€œhow can we use the high-fidelity mannequin?ā€ or ā€œwhat to do with virtual reality?ā€. The more relevant question is, ā€œwhich simulation formats are most appropriate to achieve our educational goals in our current situation (e.g., time, budget, staff availability)?ā€. The decision about particular simulation formats should deliberately balance effectiveness, efficiency, and appeal in the particular context (see also [27]).

Suppose designers seek a method to teach surgical skills and consider adding a virtual reality simulation to the program. In terms of effectiveness, the main question is: Does this VR simulator add opportunities for whole-task practice? Can learners combine nonrecurrent aspects (e.g., clinical reasoning, communications) and recurrent aspects (e.g., making incisions)? Does it increase variability of practice? Can we align the fidelity and challenge for different learners? Can it be used for automating recurrent skills? What is its added value compared to our programā€™s other instructional methods? While most VR simulators can effectively immerse the learner in an operating room, they sometimes lack haptics and tactile sensations essential for surgical interventions. Effectiveness might be high for some aspects or target groups but low for others.

Regarding efficiency, the main question is: Are there sufficient resources to add this to our program? Available budget, staffing, facilities, student group sizes, and organizational and technical support factor into the costā€“benefit analysis. Finally, in terms of appeal, the main question is: Will learnersā€”and staffā€”enjoy learning with this simulation? Appealing instructional methods may be used more often, provide motivation, and limit fatigue from redundancy in teaching methods.

By systematically considering effectiveness for whole and part-task training, efficiency, and appeal, designers can compare the strengths and weaknesses of different combinations of simulation formats in their programs. Increasing one aspect (e.g., effectiveness) in this ā€œiron triangleā€ often means sacrificing one of the other two [22]. This triangle illustrates how small changes in context, such as different levels of prior knowledge or a change in the number of students, lead to entirely different design decisions.

Recommendation 5: Optimize cognitive load

Although effective learning arises from pushing learners to the edge of their comfort zones, extreme simulation scenarios stack several unlikely complications and disasters to push learners well beyond their limits. This can easily overload learnersā€™ working memory, hampering their learning [48, 61]. The 4C/ID model is firmly based on cognitive load theory (CLT), which recommends attending to cognitive overload and ensuring that activities contribute to learning instead of detracting from it. The cognitive requirements of learning activities should not exceed working memory capacity. Cognitive load theory includes many guidelines for instructional design [17, 61] and has inspired a Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning [34] that guides the design of multimedia materials [23]. CLT has been applied to SBE to optimize cognitive load (see [17, 45, 50]), and guidelines exist to increase learning transfer [36]. We distill three important implications for the design of SBE that underpin 4C/ID design principles:

First, the intrinsic load brought on by the task itself should be managed. Whereas overly challenging tasks induce cognitive overload, boring or easy tasks (e.g., repetitive part-task training) can cause underload and subsequent learner disengagement. To avoid cognitive overload, complexity should start low and gradually increase once learners master lower-complexity tasks (i.e., a mastery approach [35]). To avoid underload, challenge can be increased by adding time pressure or introducing more whole-task practice. Second, extraneous load unrelated to the task and detrimental to learning should be minimized. Support and guide learners when complexity increases by providing modeling examples, imitation tasks, worksheets, or coaching. Avoid unclear or misleading instructions, ambiguous goals, or distractions that do not occur in real-life tasks.Footnote 1 Third, use freed-up cognitive resources to induce germane load by introducing activities that contribute to schema construction and automation, such as having learners reflect on variations of tasks (e.g., in a debriefing) and compare different strategies, or by providing cognitive feedback. These activities present desirable difficulties [6, 15]. Increasing germane load is only possible if current intrinsic and extraneous load levels remain within the bounds of the available working memory. If learners already face high demands, adding desirable difficulties will likely lead to cognitive overload and hamper learning.

We should note the challenges in measuring different types of cognitive load. Researchers use measurements such as rating scales [29, 41], dual-task methodology [42], or physiological measurements such as EEG [3] or pupillometry [54]. Educators cannot objectively monitor cognitive load but can make informed inferences. For example, failure or complaints about complexity could indicate too high intrinsic load. Insecurity, struggle, distraction, or frustration could indicate too high extraneous load. Boredom and disengagement might indicate underload. And low levels of learning and transfer despite performing authentic tasks might indicate a lack of germane load. These findings can then give the designer directions for redesigning the program.

An example blueprint

Using the 4C/ID model [55, 57], we developed a blueprint for a training program for ā€œmanaging pediatric patients with respiratory distress in the emergency departmentā€ that illustrates an application of our recommendations (see Table 1). The blueprint describes a part of a hypothetical undergraduate curriculum, including preclinical and clinical phases, and illustrates how the recommendations could be applied in one context rather than prescribing how they should be applied in all contexts.

Table 1 4C/ID training blueprint for ā€œmanaging pediatric patients with respiratory distress in the emergency departmentā€

In line with our first recommendation, the blueprint contains whole tasks requiring integration of knowledge, skills, and attitudes, and coordination of the recurrent and nonrecurrent skills shown in Fig.Ā 2. Learning tasks (i.e., 4C/ID component 1) are performed in the described order and are grouped into two levels of complexity, increasing from low to moderate, as indicated by the black rows. We also included part-task training (i.e., 4C/ID component 4), zooming in on to-be-automated recurrent skills shown in Fig.Ā 2. Part-task training follows whole-task training and is intermixed with whole tasks.

Following recommendation 2, we base this design on the outcomes of cognitive task analysis and the skill hierarchy presented in Fig.Ā 2. First, we observed and interviewed experienced practitioners, decomposed the complex skill, and labeled the skills as nonrecurrent, recurrent, or to-be-automated. Then, supportive information (i.e., 4C/ID component 2) was included for the skills labeled as nonrecurrent. This information refers to lectures, workshops, study groups, demonstrations, reading materials, digital materials, and other materials that help learners construct schemas to systematically approach tasks and support reasoning, problem-solving, and decision-making. These materials are described in the shaded rows labeled ā€œsupportive informationā€ and remain available throughout the learning program. Next, procedural information (i.e., 4C/ID component 3) was added for skills labeled ā€œrecurrentā€Ā and refers to ā€œhow-toā€ information presented in manuals for operating tools or software, reference cards, or by an instructor observing the learner and correcting mistakes. Educators present this information just-in-time during learning tasks to help learners apply rules and procedures correctly, with immediate feedback as needed. The blueprint describes this content in the cells labeled ā€œprocedural information.ā€ A CTA was essential in designing this blueprint to ensure that learning tasks were grounded in clinical practice.

Recommendation 3 focused on embedding simulation activities in broader programs. A comprehensive program includes simulations and non-simulation activities such as workshops, observations, modeling examples, or readings. Following recommendation 4, a rich mix of simulation formats is used, combining the strengths and mitigating limitations of each. This combination balances individual methodsā€™ effectiveness, efficiency, and appeal. For example, virtual patients efficiently train clinical reasoning in a low-stakes setting but complement human simulation to address communication skills and attitudes not covered in the virtual patient activities. Finally, in line with recommendation 5, appropriate levels of complexity, support, and guidance help avoid cognitive overload and desirable difficulties maximize the desired cognitive load.

This example blueprint targets final-year medical students, but training at the postgraduate level could look similar with some adaptations for dealing with complexity and scaffolding. The workplace does not always offer sufficient exposure to varying tasks for all learners, and there is often less control over task sequence and complexity. This potentially opportunistic learning can be compensated with a deliberate intertwining of workplace-based tasks and simulation tasks: simulations provide sufficient practice in a safe and supported environment, and workplace tasks prepare for independent professional practice. In the workplace, scaffolding could follow an approach based on entrustment decisions: first having learners observe workplace tasks and then gradually transitioning to performance under direct supervision, performance with supervision on request, and performance with supervision post hoc [52].

Conclusions

We provide five recommendations to inform critical design decisions in simulation programs to promote the transfer of learning. First, emphasizing whole-task practice to stimulate skill integration and coordination avoids problems of compartmentalization and fragmentation. Second, a cognitive task analysis, in addition to a needs assessment, provides a more thorough understanding of the whole complex skill and the context in which it is performed, enabling better design decisions. Third, simulation is essential but not sufficient for developing complex skills, andĀ educators should view simulation as a vital element that must be combined with other ingredients in a comprehensive learning program. Fourth, a logical combination of simulation formats should be guided by three considerations: evaluating their effectiveness for reaching different learning outcomes, their efficiency in terms of required resources, and their appeal to staff and learners in the respective context. Fifth, an optimal level of challenge can be achieved by managing the cognitive load to avoid cognitive overload or underload. When applied correctly, these five recommendations help create learning programs that provide the variation and repetition required for transfer of learning.

Implementing these recommendations may pose challenges. While theories are clear about variation and repetition, there is always tension between what is theoretically optimal and practically feasible. For example, there might not be enough time to do a CTA, not enough staff to guide all learning tasks, or not enough practice opportunities in the clinical setting. In addition, these recommendations are less relevant for short learning interventions focusing on single learning goals that do not require complex learning or do not emphasize transfer. Practical circumstances can always create reasons to deviate from these recommendations, but that should not limit innovation. A move toward more evidence-informed approaches might require a paradigm shift; we must accept that complex skills cannot be trained in a limited number of simulation sessions but require comprehensive educational programs that combine simulation sessions with workplace learning in an integrated training blueprint. These five recommendations are a first step in moving away from traditional ā€œsee one, do one, teach oneā€ approaches, muddled theories like andragogy [51], and educational myths like learning styles [28]. The full 4C/ID model offers dozens of evidence-informed guidelines for designing learning programs aimed at complex learning. For the interested reader, Van MerriĆ«nboer and Kirschner [59] provide a full description, and Table 2 presents an overview of underpinning theories.

Table 2 Theoretical underpinnings of the 4C/ID model

Transfer also involves more than good instructional design. According to studies, two other factors affect the transfer of learning [8, 33]. First, personal characteristics influence the potential for transfer, such as learnersā€™ motivation, involvement, readiness, and capacities, as well as teachersā€™ competency and teaching approach. Second, environments can vary from favorable to obstructing. This includes work environments and organizational structure (e.g., availability of workplace supervision and support), but also organizational culture and (human-resource) management (e.g., psychological safety of learners). The way these three categories of factors interact to promote or inhibit transfer depends strongly on the context.

Applications of 4C/ID and other strategies to promote transfer present exciting avenues for further research in healthcare simulation. Investigating the interplay between simulation-enhanced education design and personal and organizational factors will enhance understanding of achieving seamless transitions between simulation environments and the workplace. While many open questions remain for researchers, todayā€™s designers should integrate what is currently known about achieving transfer. We hope this paper inspires simulation educators to recognize current gaps in their approach to simulation-enhanced education while providing concrete guidance to make better-informed design choices. Just like clinical reasoning, developing instructional design skills requires time and practice. New research findings and innovations advance our field, requiring substantial investments in professionalization to remain current. Therefore, we must attend to faculty development initiatives, communities of practice, and other learning opportunities to support designers. Collaborations between universities, hospitals, clinics, simulation centers, and other research institutes at national and international levels will catalyze professional development in simulation-enhanced education. Evidence-informed approaches adopted in healthcare should translate to health professions education in general and healthcare simulation in specific.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable. The manuscript does not contain any data.

Notes

  1. Distractions that are part of real-life tasks, such as beepers going off, noisy environments, or upset family members, should be considered intrinsic load, because they are inherent to the task. These might be left out for novice learners but must be included for advanced learners.

Abbreviations

SBE:

Simulation-based education

4C/ID:

Four-component instructional design

ADDIE:

Analysis-Design-Development-Implementation-Evaluation

CTA:

Cognitive task analysis

CLT:

Cognitive load theory

References

  1. Anderson JR. How can the human mind occur in the physical universe? Oxford University Press; 2007.

    BookĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  2. Anderson RC, Spiro RJ, Anderson MC. Schemata as scaffolding for the representation of information in connected discourse. Am Educ Res J. 1978;15(3):433ā€“40. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312015003433.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Antonenko P, Paas F, Grabner R, Van Gog T. Using electroencephalography to measure cognitive load. Educ Psychol Rev. 2010;22(4):425ā€“38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9130-y.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  4. Bartlett FC. Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge University Press; 1932.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  5. Bjerrum F, Thomsen ASS, Nayahangan LJ, Konge L. Surgical simulation: current practices and future perspectives for technical skills training. Med Teach. 2018;40(7):668ā€“75. https://doi.org/10.1080/0142159X.2018.1472754.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Bjork RA. Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In: Metcalfe J, Shimamura AP, editors. Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. MIT Press; 1994. p. 185ā€“205.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  7. Bohle Carbonell K, Stalmeijer RE, Kƶnings KD, Segers M, Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG. How experts deal with novel situations: a review of adaptive expertise. Educ Res Rev. 2014;12:14ā€“29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2014.03.001.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  8. Bouzguenda K. Enablers and inhibitors of learning transfer from theory to practice. In K. Schneider (Ed.), Transfer of Learning in Organizations (pp. 23ā€“44). Springer: International Publishing; 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02093-8_3

  9. Bracq M-S, Michinov E, Jannin P. Virtual reality simulation in nontechnical skills training for healthcare professionals: a systematic review. Simul Healthc. 2019;14(3):188ā€“94. https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0000000000000347.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  10. Clark RE. Cognitive task analysis for expert-based instruction in health care. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. Elen, & M. J. Bishop (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Educational Communications and Technology (4th ed., pp. 541ā€“551). Springer; 2014.

  11. Clark RE, Pugh CM, Yates KA, Inaba K, Green DJ, Sullivan ME. The use of cognitive task analysis to improve instructional descriptions of procedures. J Surg Res. 2012;173(1):e37ā€“42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jss.2011.09.003.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  12. Cook DA, Triola MM. Virtual patients: a critical literature review and proposed next steps. Med Educ. 2009;43(4):303ā€“11. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03286.x.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  13. Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: a general overview. Acad Emerg Med. 2008;15(11):988ā€“94. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2008.00227.x.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Ericsson KA, Lehmann AC. Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annu Rev Psychol. 1996;47(1):273ā€“305. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.273.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  15. Eva KW. Diagnostic error in medical education: where wrongs can make rights. Adv Health Sci Educ. 2009;14(S1):71ā€“81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-009-9188-9.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Francom GM. Principles for task-centered instruction. In C. M. Reigeluth, B. J. Beatty, & R. D. Myers (Eds.), Instructional-design theories and models (Vol. 4, pp. 65ā€“92).Ā New York: Routledge; 2016.

  17. Fraser KL, Ayres P, Sweller J. Cognitive load theory for the design of medical simulations. Simul Healthc. 2015;10(5):295ā€“307. https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0000000000000097.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  18. Frerejean J, Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Dolmans DHJM. Research on instructional design in the health sciences: from taxonomies of learning to whole-task models. In J. Cleland & S. J. Durning (Eds.), Researching medical education (2nd ed., pp. 291ā€“302). Wiley; 2022.

  19. Frerejean J, Van Merriƫnboer JJG, Kirschner PA, Roex A, Aertgeerts B, Marcellis M. Designing instruction for complex learning: 4C/ID in higher education. Eur J Educ. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejed.12363.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Gaba DM. The future vision of simulation in healthcare. Simul Healthc. 2007;2(2):126ā€“35. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.SIH.0000258411.38212.32.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Grossman R, Salas E. The transfer of training: what really matters: The transfer of training. Int J Train Dev. 2011;15(2):103ā€“20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2419.2011.00373.x.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  22. Honebein PC, Honebein CH. Effectiveness, efficiency, and appeal: pick any two? The influence of learning domains and learning outcomes on designer judgments of useful instructional methods. Educ Tech Res Dev. 2015;63(6):937ā€“55. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-015-9396-3.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. Issa N, Schuller M, Santacaterina S, Shapiro M, Wang E, Mayer RE, DaRosa DA. Applying multimedia design principles enhances learning in medical education: Multimedia design principles to enhance learning. Med Educ. 2011;45(8):818ā€“26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2011.03988.x.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  24. Jonassen DH. Cognitive flexibility theory and its implications for designing CBI. In S. Dijkstra, H. P. M. Krammer, & J. J. G. Van MerriĆ«nboer (Eds.), Instructional models in computer-based learning environments (NATO ASI Series F) (Vol. 104, pp. 385ā€“403).Ā Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 1992.

  25. Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. 1st ed. Straus and Giroux: Farrar; 2011.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  26. Kester L, Kirschner PA, Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Baumer A. Just-in-time information presentation and the acquisition of complex cognitive skills. Comput Hum Behav. 2001;17(4):373ā€“91. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0747-5632(01)00011-5.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Khan K, Tolhurst-Cleaver S, White S, Simpson W. Simulation in healthcare education. Building a simulation programme: a practical guide.Ā Dundee: Association for Medical Education in Europe (AMEE); 2011.

  28. Kirschner PA. Stop propagating the learning styles myth. Comput Educ. 2017;106:166ā€“71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2016.12.006.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Leppink J, Paas F, Van der Vleuten CPM, Van Gog T, Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG. Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behav Res Methods. 2013;45(4):1058ā€“72. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0334-1.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  30. Lim J, Reiser RA, Olina Z. The effects of part-task and whole-task instructional approaches on acquisition and transfer of a complex cognitive skill. Educ Tech Res Dev. 2009;57:61ā€“77. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-007-9085-y.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  31. Lowrey W, Kim KS. Online news media and advanced learning: a test of cognitive flexibility theory. J Broadcast Electron Media. 2009;53(4):547ā€“66. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838150903323388.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  32. Marton F. Necessary conditions of learning. Routledge; 2015.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  33. Masoomi R, Shariati M, Labaf A, Mirzazadeh A. Transfer of learning from simulated setting to the clinical setting: identifying instructional design features. Med J The Islamic Republic of Iran. 2021.Ā https://doi.org/10.47176/mjiri.35.90

  34. Mayer RE (Ed.). The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (Second Edition).Ā Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2014.

  35. McGaghie WC, Barsuk JH, Wayne DB. (Eds.).Ā Comprehensive healthcare simulation: mastery learning in health professions education. Springer International Publishing; 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-34811-3

  36. Meguerdichian M, Walker K, Bajaj K. Working memory is limited: improving knowledge transfer by optimising simulation through cognitive load theory. BMJ Simul Technol Enhanc Learn. 2016;2(4):131ā€“8. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjstel-2015-000098.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  37. Merrill MD. First principles of instruction: identifying and designing effective, efficient, and engaging instruction.Ā Bloomington: Association for Educational Communications and Technology (AECT); 2020.

  38. Nestel D, Groom J, Eikeland-HusebĆø S, Oā€™Donnell JM. Simulation for learning and teaching procedural skills: the state of the science. Simul Healthc. 2011;6(7):S10ā€“3. https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0b013e318227ce96.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  39. Nestel D, Tierney T. Role-play for medical students learning about communication: guidelines for maximising benefits. BMC Med Educ. 2007;7(1):3. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-7-3.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  40. Olsen SE, Rasmussen J. The reflective expert and the prenovice: notes on skill-, rule- and knowledge-based performance in the setting of instruction and training. In: Bainbridge L, Quintanilla SAR, editors. Developing skills with information technology. Wiley; 1989. p. 9ā€“33.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  41. Paas FGWC. Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: a cognitive-load approach. J Educ Psychol. 1992;84(4):429ā€“34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  42. Park B, BrĆ¼nken R. The rhythm method: a new method for measuring cognitive load-an experimental dual-task study. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2015;29(2):232ā€“43. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3100.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  43. Peters S, Clarebout G, Van Nuland M, Aertgeerts B, Roex A. How to connect classroom and workplace learning. Clin Teach. 2017;14(5):313ā€“8. https://doi.org/10.1111/tct.12697.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  44. Ragan TJ, Smith PL, Curda LK. Outcome-referenced, conditions-based theories and models. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. J. G. Van Merrienboer, & M. P. Driscoll (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Educational Communications and Technology (3rd ed., pp. 383ā€“400). Erlbaum/Routledge; 2008.

  45. Reedy GB. Using cognitive load theory to inform simulation design and practice. Clin Simul Nurs. 2015;11(8):355ā€“60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecns.2015.05.004.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  46. Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review. 1977;84(1):1ā€“66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  47. Snell L, Son D, Onishi H. Instructional design: applying theory to teaching practice. In T. Swanwick, K. Forrest, & B. C. Oā€™Brien (Eds.), Understanding Medical Education (pp. 89ā€“100). Wiley; 2018.Ā https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119373780.ch6

  48. Sweller J, Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Paas F. Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educ Psychol Rev. 2019;31(2):261ā€“92. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  49. Sweller J, Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Paas FGWC. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ Psychol Rev. 1998;10(3):251ā€“96. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022193728205.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  50. Szulewski A, Howes D, van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Sweller J. From theory to practice: the application of cognitive load theory to the practice of medicine. Acad Med. 2021;96(1):24ā€“30. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0000000000003524.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  51. Taylor B, Kroth M. Andragogyā€™s transition into the future: Meta-analysis of andragogy and its search for a measurable instrument. J Adult Educ. 2009;38(1):1ā€“11.

  52. Ten Cate O. Nuts and bolts of entrustable professional activities. J Grad Med Educ. 2013;5(1):157ā€“8. https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-12-00380.1.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  53. Tjiam IM, Schout BM, Hendrikx AJ, Scherpbier AJ, Witjes JA, Van Merrienboer JJG. Designing simulator-based training: an approach integrating cognitive task analysis and four-component instructional design. Med Teach. 2012;34:698ā€“707. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2012.687480.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  54. Van Gerven PWM, Paas F, Van Merrienboer JJG, Schmidt HG. Memory load and the cognitive pupillary response in aging. Psychophysiology. 2004;41(2):167ā€“74. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2003.00148.x.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  55. Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG. Training complex cognitive skills: a four-component instructional design model for technical training.Ā Englewood Cliffs: Educational Technology Publications; 1997.

  56. Van MerriĆ«nboer, J. J. G. (2012). Variability of practice. In N. M. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning (pp. 3389ā€“3390). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_415

  57. Van Merriƫnboer, J. J. G. (2019). The four-component instructional design model. An overview of its main design principles. Maastricht University. https://www.4cid.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/04/vanmerrienboer-4cid-overview-of-main-design-principles-2021.pdf

  58. Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Jelsma O, Paas FGWC. Training for reflective expertise: a four-component instructional design model for complex cognitive skills. Educ Tech Res Dev. 1992;40(2):23ā€“43. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02297047.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  59. Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Kirschner PA. Ten steps to complex learning: a systematic approach to four-component instructional design (3rd ed.).Ā New York: Routledge; 2018.

  60. Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Sweller J. Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educ Psychol Rev. 2005;17(2):147ā€“77. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  61. Van MerriĆ«nboer JJG, Sweller J. Cognitive load theory in health professional education: design principles and strategies. Med Educ. 2010;44(1):85ā€“93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03498.x.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  62. Wickens CD, Hutchins S, Carolan T, Cumming J. Effectiveness of part-task training and increasing-difficulty training strategies: a meta-analysis approach. Hum Factors. 2013;55(2):461ā€“70. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720812451994.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Not applicable. This theoretical paper was not funded.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JF acted as the first author of the manuscript. All authors provided several rounds of feedback on the text, the skill hierarchy, and the educational blueprint. After many revisions, all authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jimmy Frerejean.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable. The manuscript does not report any studies involving human participants, human data, human tissue, or animals.

Consent for publication

Not applicable. The manuscript does not contain data from any individual person.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisherā€™s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Frerejean, J., van Merriƫnboer, J.J.G., Condron, C. et al. Critical design choices in healthcare simulation education: a 4C/ID perspective on design that leads to transfer. Adv Simul 8, 5 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41077-023-00242-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41077-023-00242-7

Keywords