Skip to main content

TIDES: examining the influence of temporal individual differences on multitasking in educational simulation

A Correction to this article was published on 20 December 2023

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

The majority of tasks nurses complete in acute care settings are time-sensitive. Due to complex patient needs, nurses’ multitasking behavior is of growing importance. Situations involving multitasking behavior typically require nurses to switch their attention among multiple tasks and patients in a rapid fashion. Research suggests temporal individual differences such as time urgency, polychronicity, and time perspective influence decision-making. The factors suggest that balanced time perspective may facilitate multitasking. Given novice nurses commit errors related to multitasking, we evaluated the relationship between temporal individual differences, cognitive workload, and multitasking behaviors in a simulation setting.

Methods

A one-group repeated measures design was used to evaluate the relationship between multitasking, demographic factors, cognitive workload, and temporal individual differences. One hundred sixty fourth-year, prelicensure nursing students independently completed two 45-min multiple patients simulations involving care of three interactive patient simulators. Participants completed the Multitasking Preference Inventory, Time Perspective Inventory, Experiences of Time survey, and Time Urgency Scale before simulation. A summary Creighton Simulation Evaluation Instrument score was used to represent multitasking. Participants completed the Task Load Index to represent cognitive workload. We calculated deviation from balanced time perspective and measured its correlation with multitasking. Regression models calculated how much variance deviation from balanced time perspective, demographic factors, and cognitive workload contributed to multitasking.

Results

Standardized test scores were more predictive of multitasking than deviation from balanced time perspective (β = 0.19, t = 2.48, p = 0.0142). As deviation from balanced time perspective increased, multitasking behaviors decreased (r = − 0.17), participants reported a higher sense of urgency (r = 0.39), and they had more frustration after simulation (r = 0.22). Deviation from balanced time perspective did not influence cognitive workload.

Conclusions

Nursing students who demonstrate multitasking behaviors tend to have a more balanced time perspective. Knowing students’ deviation from balanced time perspective may help educators anticipate who will need more assistance with multitasking in simulation. Nursing students frequently wait until just before graduation to provide care for multiple patients; including mention of deviation from balanced time perspective in simulation preparation may help senior nursing students become more self-aware and ultimately improve behavioral performance.

Background

The majority of tasks nurses complete in acute care settings are time-sensitive—especially assessment and medication administration at change of shift. Nurses’ multitasking behavior and strategies to improve multitasking are of growing importance related to patients’ complex health needs [1]. Large numbers of novice nurses enter the workforce each year, and because nurses’ work is time-sensitive, it is important for novice nurses to switch their attention among multiple tasks in order to manage multiple patients’ assessments, medication administration, and procedures. Multitasking in the context of nursing care delivery represents both cognitive and psychomotor skills. It is difficult to understand why novice nurses may fail to recognize deteriorating patient conditions and how to ascertain whether they are completing the task of assessment versus interpreting assessment data [2]. Novice nurses are known to commit medication errors related to wrong dose, wrong route, and administration technique [3]. Fifty percent of all medical errors involve novice nurses with less than 1-year experience [4]. Existing research indicates the potential for significant negative effects on decision-making outcomes from stress induced by perceived time pressure [5], and time pressure perceptions are in turn influenced by temporal individual differences [6]. Given that novice nurses commit more errors related to individual tasks and to multitasking in time-sensitive patient situations [7], we chose here to evaluate the relationship between temporal individual differences and multitasking behaviors in a controlled, simulation laboratory setting.

Multitasking is inextricably linked to how individuals react to and regard time [8]. Nurses usually provide care for between four to seven patients at a time over the course of a 12-h shift [9]. Multitasking situations typically require nurses to switch their attention among multiple tasks and patients in a rapid manner [7, 10]. Nurses starting a day shift, for example, typically assess their four to seven patients, prioritize pro re nata medications like insulin before breakfast, and administer daily medications all during the first two hours of their shift; nurses may plan 5 min to complete each task per patient, but they are often interrupted at the start of shift with new laboratory results, orders, and interprofessional rounds. As a result, many nurses perceive inadequate time resources in which to complete their work. In situations of time scarcity, temporal individual differences—that is stable individual traits pertaining to how we perceive, consider and react to time—may become important predictors of multitasking behavior [11]. Three such differences have received considerable attention in the management and social psychology literatures over the past two decades: time urgency, polychronicity, and time perspective.

Time urgency is a temporal individual difference and subcomponent of the type A behavior pattern [12, 13] associated with time-related behaviors such as scheduling, list making, and hurrying (see Table 1). Polychronicity is an individual’s preference for doing multiple tasks simultaneously [15], whereas multitasking represents the behavioral aspect of a polychronicity preference. Finally, time perspective is a bias in the temporal frames—past, present, or future—used by an individual in tasks such as planning and decision making [16]. For example, in advising novice trainees, a mentor with a more “past time perspective” might provide substantially more examples from their own past to show how procedures and values have changed, while a mentor with a more “present time perspective” might encourage trainees to focus and reflect on their own current experiences; in contrast, a mentor with a more “future time perspective” might emphasize the importance of thinking ahead and planning next steps more often. Because of the present and future orientation of most work tasks in organizations, research on time perspective has focused on present and future orientations [17]. While being oriented to notice the passage of time (i.e., time urgent) and preferring to perform multiple tasks simultaneously (i.e., polychronic) are individual differences facilitating the switching among several tasks, a general orientation toward the future rather than present may allow individuals to better plan for multitasking; conversely, a more present orientation could lead individuals to become more single-task focused or “lost in the moment” and less able to multitask [18]. Further, individuals with a balanced time perspective (BTP)—that is, relatively equal levels of all past, present, and future time perspectives and the ability to switch among the perspectives to better fit situational demands—could be the most flexible and able to multitask at the appropriate moments [8, 19]. Table 2 depicts specific characteristics associated with temporal individual differences.

Table 1 Behavior pattern characteristics [14]
Table 2 Characteristics associated with temporal individual differences [12, 15, 16, 20]

Researchers have used time perspective to predict both psychosocial outcomes and behavior in undergraduate students from female-dominated classes similar to nursing [21]. Recent theoretical work suggests that individuals with BTP are able to attain a higher level of situational awareness than temporally skewed counterparts by switching among temporal foci as dynamic situations change—an ability hypothesized to facilitate multitasking [8]. More specifically, calculating a deviation from balanced time perspective (DBTP) as a measure of fit [20, 22] most accurately represents the nature of a balanced time perspective. A DBTP value close to zero indicates the theoretical ideal of a near balanced time perspective, where a large positive value indicates time perspective is skewed and expected to be maladaptive [20]. The shape of a DBTP curve is expected to be parabolic. In the setting of individuals planning for retirement and future behavior, those with a DBTP close to zero are able to make future plans informed by an evaluation of past experiences and current lifestyle, while individuals with a higher DBTP would consider their present lifestyle with more weight [23]. In general, having an unbalanced or skewed time perspective leaves one vulnerable when task situation demands fail to match one’s constrained time perspective preference and behaviors; conversely, having a balanced time perspective allows one to adapt time perspective and behavior to fit demands of the situation at hand.

The simulation laboratory is an ideal setting to examine the influence of temporal individual differences on multitasking behaviors. Simulation mimics the reality of a clinical environment providing novice nurses with opportunities to demonstrate critical thinking, decision-making, and behavioral procedures [24]. In multiple patients simulation, we can recognize nursing students’ preferred work speed to an extent by their low behavioral performance scores in a time-sensitive scenarioFootnote 1. There is a clear role for assessment of nursing student performance in simulation [25], and confounding variables related to temporal individual differences may help us understand why simulation-based education is effective. It is common for researchers to look for relationships between simulation performance and demographic variables including age, grade point average (GPA), and learning style preference. In meta-analyses, it is clear that simulation-based education has substantial effects on healthcare providers’ behavior change compared to no intervention [26], and simulation-based education is superior to non-simulation-based training to impact skills and behaviors [27]. Both of these meta-analyses found heterogeneity in the results; however, suggesting simulation activities vary in degrees of effectiveness. As such, simulationists seek to understand mechanisms underlying differences in simulation effectiveness (i.e., What makes simulation-based education work better? [24]). Since 2010, simulation researchers have sought to understand how to use the right type of simulation with the right learner and the right objective at the right time [28, 29]. This study seeks to understand how to use multiple patients simulation with fourth year, prelicensure nursing students and examine how temporal individual differences contribute to effective multitasking as the “right objective.”

During educational simulation, students have opportunities to practice multitasking in a realistic environment without potential danger to patients. Multiple patients simulation offers novice nurses a safe place to practice managing multiple responsibilities. Previous research indicates that multiple patients simulation increases nursing students’ motivation and self-efficacy for providing independent nursing care [30]. A gap in the literature persists however in understanding factors underpinning improved behavioral performance. As a result, we do not know whether nursing students learning how to “do simulation” over time, if facilitation skills and debriefing contribute more to improved behavioral performance than simulation practice, or if there are personality traits or other factors contributing to effective multitasking.

This descriptive study addressed the following research questions: “Is deviation from balanced time perspective more predictive of multitasking behavior in multiple patients simulation compared to demographic factors or cognitive workload in novice nurses?” and “What are the relationships between novice nurses’ deviation from balanced time perspective and their polychronicity, time urgency, flexibility, and frustration after multiple patients simulation?”

Methods

We used a one-group repeated measures design to examine the impact of temporal individual differences on multitasking behaviors in two 45-min multiple patients simulations in which participants worked independently. This study was conducted in the simulation laboratory at a nursing school in the Southwestern United States. Researchers recruited a convenience sample of 164 fourth-year, prelicensure nursing students enrolled in a capstone clinical course. All nursing students were invited to participate, and there were no exclusion criteria. Prior to this simulation, all students completed 12 group simulations during the previous two semesters. Participation in the study had no effect on course grades. This investigation was approved by the Institutional Review Board (1806-109-1807) where the study took place and was conducted in accordance with the tenets espoused in the Declaration of Helsinki [31].

Data collection

Participants completed surveys on a Qualtrics XM (Provo, UT, USA) platform after having provided consent. Participants self-reported demographic data, while researchers collected GPA and Assessment Technologies Institute (ATI) standardized test scores from academic records. Participants completed the Multitasking Preference Inventory [32], Time Perspective Inventory [16], Experiences of Time survey [33], and the Time Urgency Scale [34] in a 30-min session immediately following consent and on a separate day than simulation. The scales have evidence of reliability and evidence of validity with diverse samples of college students from the USA [12, 33], UK [35], and the Middle East [21]. Use of scales with multiple items and some reverse coding limits the bias of demand characteristics where participants may “second guess” and thus be motivated to fulfill (or thwart) study purposes.

After simulation and before debriefing, participants used the Qualtrics platform to complete the six-item National Aeronautics and Space Administration Task Load Index (NASA TLX) to measure cognitive workload [36]. The TLX requires participants to rate mental and physical demand alongside the amount of effort they exerted, success in their performance, and level of frustration. Participants self-rate on a scale of 1–20. Researchers used item-level scores for analysis of frustration and a global summary score to represent cognitive workload. The TLX also has evidence of reliability and evidence of validity [37]. Table 3 provides a summary of the constructs assessed with each measure.

Table 3 Summary of measures

Multiple patients scenario and scoring performance behaviors

A complete description of the multiple patients scenario is available in a previous publication [38]. Briefly, the scenario involved care of three interactive patient simulators for 45-min at the beginning of shift. Educators provided a scripted orientation to the environment, and had participants had 10 min to review the electronic health record independently after receiving report. One patient required rescue medications including pro re nata medications for chest pain. Participants completed the scenario independently, and there were no observers. Table 4 provides details about the multiple patients scenario. Given the inherent time constraints of the beginning of shift, participants were required to establish and reorganize priorities. Debriefing was not included in data collection or analysis, though educators used a standard script with open-ended prompts (see Additional file 1 Appendix) and stated behavioral observations guided by the Creighton Simulation Evaluation Instrument™ (CSEI) using the Advocacy/Inquiry approach. The majority of participants completed both simulations before starting their capstone shifts which involved total patient care.

Table 4 Key elements of multiple patients scenario

Performance behaviors were scored using the CSEI [39]. The instrument has evidence of validity, reliability, and interrater reliability [40]. Scores are calculated by summing the responses, and higher scores on the CSEI represent effective multitasking. Eight educators rated participants’ multiple patients simulation, and a research assistant verified interrater reliability in a random ten percent of the sample. Gwet’s AC2 calculation for inter-rater reliability was used.

Deviation from balanced time perspective

Five time perspective factors are measured with the 56-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory [16]. Table 5 depicts sample items from each of the five time perspective factors. DBTP allows researchers to categorize participants individually based on their distance from a balanced time perspective profile [22]. Balanced time perspectives optimize how individuals meet situational demands of life and how they think, and how they behave. We used a formula to calculate DBTP using subscales of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory:

$$ DBTP=\sqrt{{\left( oPN- ePN\right)}^2+{\left( oPP- ePP\right)}^2+{\left( oPF- ePF\right)}^2+{\left( oPH- ePH\right)}^2+{\left( oF- eF\right)}^2} $$
Table 5 Factors and sample questions on Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory [16, 20]

where oPN is the optimal past negative subscale,

ePN is the empirical past negative subscale,

oPP is the optimal past positive subscale,

ePP is the empirical past positive subscale,

oPF is the optimal present fatalism subscale,

ePF is the empirical present fatalism subscale,

oPH is the optimal present hedonism subscale,

ePH is the empirical present hedonism subscale,

oF is the optimal futurism subscale, and

eF is the empirical futurism subscale [20]

After calculating the deviation from balanced time perspective, it was operationalized in this investigation as a categorical variable based on tertiles (0 to 1.518030, > 1.518030 to 2.038265; and > 2.038265).

Analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated—means (standard deviations; SD) for quantitative data and frequencies and percentages (relative frequencies) for qualitative data. Dependent variables were tested for normality using normal probability plots and the Anderson-Darling, Shapiro-Francia, and the Shapiro-Wilk normality tests. Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient was used to assess the bivariate relationships [41].

Analysis of variance was used to determine if DBTP was predictive of CSEI scores. The preferred method, the Tukey-Kramer method, was used to control the comparisonwise error rate for post hoc pairwise comparisons. The paired t test or the Wilcoxon Signed-rank test (based on the results of normality testing) was used to assess if the first CSEI score was an important predictor of the second CSEI score. After which, analysis of covariance was used to assess of DBTP has any effect on second CSEI scores after adjusting for initial score. Linear regression was used to measure the effects of GPA, ATI, DBTP, and cognitive workload as measured by the NASA TLX. In regression models, DBTP was entered as a quantitative variable.

The family of kappa measures for inter-rater reliability have documented problems [42,43,44]. To resolve these paradoxes, Gwet proposed a family of inter-rater reliability statistics; the AC statistics [45]. Gwet’s AC1 is a measure of inter-rater reliability for nominal data for multiple raters while Gwet’s AC2 is a measure of inter-rater reliability for ordinal and interval measurement [46]. Gwet’s AC2 calculation for inter-rater reliability was used. All analyses were done using software for statistical computing (R v.3.5.2).

Results

Table 6 depicts demographic characteristics of the sample comparing participants in three groups based on their calculated DBTP. Four participants had missing data related to surveys completed after simulation and were not included in analysis; 160 participants completed the study protocol. Data were judged to be normally distributed except for a difference between first CSEI score and second CSEI simulation score; therefore, the second CSEI simulation score was rank transformed for analysis. Inter-rater reliability (Gwet’s AC2) for the 10% random sample of CSEI was .78 (95% CI .76–.80).

Table 6 Demographics of DBTP groups

Research question 1

As DBTP increased, participants’ CSEI scores decreased in the first simulation (r = − 0.17). In regression models adjusting for GPA, ATI, DBTP, and second NASA TLX score, results were similar. On the first multiple patients simulation, only ATI score predicted CSEI scores in a meaningfulFootnote 2 way. Approximately every one-point change in ATI score will be associated with a five-point change in the CSEI score (β = 0.19, t = 2.48, p = 0.0142). A model including GPA, ATI, DBTP, and NASA TLX score explained about 14% of the variance on CSEI score in the initial multiple patients simulation.

On the second multiple patients simulation, ATI score and DBTP predicted simulation performance scores on the CSEI. Approximately every one-point change in ATI score will be associated with a five-point change in the CSEI score (β = 0.18, t = 2.64, p = 0.009). For every five-point increase in DBTP, there was a corresponding six-point decrease in CSEI score (β = − 1.23, t = − 1.91, p = 0.059). A model including GPA, ATI, DBTP, and score on the NASA TLX explained 13% of the variance on CSEI score in the second multiple patients simulation.

Regardless of DBTP group assignment (low, medium, high), there was no meaningful difference in performance on the second multiple patients simulation performance (where participants were familiar with the environment and objectives; F(2, 157) = 1.703, p = 0.186). A Wilcoxon signed rank test demonstrated a meaningful difference between multitasking on the first and second simulations (p < 0.0001, r = 0.09). However, in an analysis of covariance of DBTP adjusting for first CSEI score, DBTP was not a predictor of the second CSEI score (p = 0.400). Adjusting for DBTP category did not modify the results of the model, initial CSEI score was still predictive of second CSEI score at the same level (p < 0.001). However, a model of DBTP and first CSEI score accounted for about 28% of the variance in the second CSEI score.

DBTP was not predictive of CSEI score. Further, DBTP did not substantially influence CSEI score or cognitive workload. However, in both simulations, ATI score was an important or meaningful predictor of CSEI score with every one-point change accounting for a five to six-point change in CSEI score. Both adjusted models accounted for minimal variance suggesting there are additional variables at play.

Research question 2

Participants with a preference for multitasking on the Multitasking Preference Inventory [32] also reported not having urgency in responding to everyday situations (r = 0.25). As DBTP increased, participants reported a higher sense of urgency on the Time Urgency Scale [12] and frustration after their first multiple patients simulation (see Table 7). Physiological correlations (e.g., amount of nicotine administered to the heart and heart rate) of this magnitude may be considered low; however, behavioral indications correlations (e.g., multitasking and temporal demand) of this magnitude can be considered moderate. There were minimal relationships detected between multitasking and cognitive workload at simulation 1 (r = 0.14) and simulation 2 (r = 0.11). Regardless of DBTP group assignment (low, medium, high), there was no difference in cognitive workload after the second simulation (F(2, 155) = 0.6963, p = 0.5).

Table 7 Temporal individual difference correlations

Discussion

Nurse educators use simulation for teaching and assessment to mitigate the gap between academics and independent nursing practice. For years, employers have provided academic nurse educators with feedback that novice nurses are ill-prepared for practice and cannot multitask or manage their time appropriately [48, 49]. This is the first study to investigate how nursing students multitask in multiple patients simulation and to examine how temporal individual differences, demographic factors, and cognitive workload relate to multitasking behaviors. Findings from this study of 160 novice nursing students indicate that DBTP was not predictive of multitasking and ATI standardized test scores were actually more predictive. DBTP did not influence cognitive workload. Regression models accounted for minimal variance suggesting variables other than DBTP, demographic factors, and cognitive workload influence novice nurses’ multitasking.

This study builds on the existing multiple patients simulation body of knowledge by identifying predictors of multitasking. Seven research teams have previously published multiple patients studies where simulation was a platform to evaluate multitasking behavior, though this study was the first to examine the relationship between multitasking behaviors, demographic factors, and nursing students’ temporal individual differences. In earlier trials, novice nurses improved in competent behavioral performance with additional practice in multiple patients simulations [38, 50, 51]. Repeated multiple patients simulation opportunities improved nursing students’ competent behavioral performance, though manipulating simulation preparation activities did not further enhance behavioral performance [52]. Findings from this study are similar as novice nurses’ multitasking behavior improves with repeated exposure in simulation; this study was unique because educators facilitated debriefing after simulation (see Additional file 1 Appendix), and that may have contributed to the meaningful difference in multitasking between simulations 1 and 2. Because hearing an expert’s observations and practicing components of a specific skill repeatedly improve behavior, educators in academic and practice settings continue to advance the platform of simulation-based education to improve healthcare providers’ performance through promoting skill acquisition, uncovering clinical variations, maintaining competence, and building efficiency in care delivery processes [25].

One of the largest challenges for nursing students to overcome is learning to manage multiple responsibilities simultaneously and independently [53]. In order to preserve patient safety, nursing students usually provide care to only one patient at a time during clinical courses. Providing care to one patient at a time helps new nurses think deeply about their patient’s condition, medications, and treatments. Although this deep, critical thinking helps student nurses develop core thinking skills, one of the downsides to only providing care to one patient at a time is it does not motivate completion of multiple tasks simultaneously. However, completing multiple tasks simultaneously is a characteristic of real-world independent nursing practice, especially because nurses provide care to four to seven patients simultaneously in the majority of acute care settings and multitask 34% of their shift (range 23–41% [54]).

We anticipated that nursing students with a balanced time perspective would multitask more effectively. Existing literature from psychology and organizational behavior indicates substantial influence of temporal individual differences on work behaviors, ranging from differences in individuals’ preferred work speeds [55], to planning for the future [56], and suggests polychronicity [57] and time urgency [58] are correlated with multitasking behavior. Although past time perspective is hypothesized to be associated with a monochronic preference [18], and both present and future time perspectives are theorized to influence reactions to working under deadlines [17], we are aware of no empirical work examining the relationship between time perspective and actual multitasking behavior in a nursing context.

Even though multitasking behaviors increased over time, our participants’ scores related to priority setting and assessment are still poor. Namely, participants earned only 29% of priority setting points and only 50% of assessment points on their second multiple patients simulation. These results are actually much worse than those reported by Kavanaugh and Szweda with a much larger sample of new graduate novice nurses using a computerized assessment of priority setting skills [53]. It could be our experiential multiple patients simulations where nursing students physically perform assessments, give medications, and respond to interactive patient simulators in a realistic setting may uncover more discrepancies related to priority setting and assessment than computerized simulation exercises. Similarly, it seems GPA may represent intellectual ability but not relate to multitasking behavioral performance. Extant literature also highlights the role of novice nurses’ values and how a preference for listening to patients and providing them with explanations about the care plan may be more important to novice nurses with 6 months of practice experience compared to nursing students in the present study [48]. Our research team observed that nursing students tended to favor completing tasks in a linear fashion (e.g., repeating the same assessment on several patients without individualizing the care plan to patient history) and to value interventions in lieu of prioritizing care. However, 75% of the points on the CSEI in the priority setting section required patient teaching about priorities of care and explanations of rationale for individualized patient goals and interventions.

Data from the NASA TLX revealed a weak correlation between level of frustration and a sense of urgency and DBTP. Though a level of frustration seems like a negative affective outcome following simulation, previous research validates how novice nurses vacillate between feeling frustrated and confident even within the confines of a single 12-h shift [48]. To our research team, levels of students’ frustration were appropriate in the setting of time scarcity and multitasking. Previous qualitative research with new graduate nurses referred to a continuum of frustration to confidence [48]; because confidence after simulation routinely does not match competent behavioral performance [59], frustration seems to be an acceptable representation of challenge and reaction to complex educational simulations. Further, because our participants completed the NASA TLX before debriefing, it is important to recognize how debriefing after difficult clinical situations naturally deescalates frustration as students reflect both on judgments underpinning behavior as well as opportunities for improvement [60].

Our findings are similar to published research about new graduate nurses and multitasking. Reflecting on their transition to practice as new graduate nurses, it is common for novices to report apprehension about completing all of their tasks and making a mistake contributes to the struggle they experience with time management in the early months of independent practice [61]. Further, novice nurses describe a tension between “being able to do time management and patient safety” [61] potentially explaining the frustration our participants experienced in a time-sensitive multiple patients simulation. Interestingly, however, this tension around multitasking seems to resolve for nurses who have practiced independently for a while. For example, researchers found that nurses view multitasking as a prerequisite for providing care in an Emergency Department because multitasking implies efficiency [62]. Further, expert nurses have identified the relationship between multitasking and patient error is perceived to be mediated by who the nurse is and their level of experience [62]. As such, it is prudent to consider lack of experience for new graduate nurses and our nursing students contributes to frustration in multiple patients simulation.

Our findings related to DBTP and multitasking are not to be considered an impasse as a research outcome. It could be one reason we did not find DBTP as predictive of multitasking relates to measurement and use of dichotomous checklists to capture multitasking behavior. Though the CSEI is widely used in simulation research and works well because nurse educators can individualize behavioral descriptors to their scenario and objectives [39, 40], the dichotomous nature of the tool may limit conclusions about multitasking behavior. More specifically, the CSEI does not account for frequency or timing of behaviors in the manner than more fluid time motion studies potentially represent [7, 10]. Further, our research team noted lack of weighting on CSEI items limits conclusions about priority setting. For example, the CSEI awards equal points for foundational skills and priority setting decisions even though the two represent varied levels of complexity. From a research perspective, it would be helpful to ascertain whether or not nursing students multitask effectively while also demonstrating higher-order priority setting skills. It is worthwhile to consider that most associations between DBTP and cognitive functions are relatively small [63], so refining the measure of multitasking in simulation and re-examining the relationship in future study is warranted.

By examining the influence of temporal individual differences in the realistic and consequential setting of the nursing simulation laboratory, we have started a conversation about predictors of effective multitasking for nursing students. Previous research has shown that “future time perspective” mediates the relationship between negative awareness of age-related change and psychological well-being in adults; the more expansive the “future time perspective,” the more healthy the sense of well-being [64]. Similarly, if a nursing student knows their DBTP is high, they might choose to gain more multitasking practice, focus more intently on task switching skill development, or seek a work environment requiring less time-pressured multitasking behavior. Simulation facilitators may find it helpful to use more Advocacy/Inquiry in debriefing with students who have a high DBTP to facilitate a higher level of self-reflection. Nurse educators can use DBTP to screen nursing students and novice nurses and in turn help nurses become more aware of their own strengths and limitations related to BTP so they are able to work independently and have a sense of well-being. The ultimate goal is to create targeted interventions helping nursing students overcome individual challenges with multitasking in order to decrease error and improve patient safety. Considering data on self-reported perceptions of temporal difference alongside rater observation of multitasking behavior, results seems promising. The next steps in this line of research are to add behavioral process into evaluation to uncover how temporal individual differences influence performance outcomes. Incorporating time motion studies to code simulation videos for the presence and frequency of behaviors should add to our understanding of factors contributing to multitasking in simulation.

This study has several educational implications. Nurse educators can use Zimbardo’s Time Perspective Inventory [16] and calculate DBTP [22] as a screening tool to understand nursing students’ time perspective balance. Information about time perspective balance combined with data on academic performance, computerized assessments of higher-order thinking skills, and simulation can help educators create an individualized education plan for at-risk nursing students and novice nurses. Educators in practice settings already use simulation to improve novice nurses’ management, priority setting, and assessment skills [48, 65]. Adding a survey of temporal individual differences may assist educators to recognize how stable traits impact nursing care and priority setting.

Nursing students preferring to work more slowly seem to revert to a comfort zone of technical skills (e.g., patient identification and vital signs) rather than taking initiative to prioritize assessment, decide what data requires the most urgent response, and follow through to deliver appropriate patient care. These students seem paralyzed or “lost in the moment” by the daunting list of tasks when managing multiple responsibilities. As a result, novice nurses, generally, are less able to assess a clinical situation or ask pertinent assessment questions enabling them to recognize a change in patient condition [66]. In planning multiple patients simulations to foster multitasking, educators should design scenarios rewarding nursing students’ assessment and priority setting in lieu of interventions in order to reinforce the unique position of nurses to recognize cues of a change in patient status and alter care plans accordingly. Further, educators should encourage nursing students to communicate their priorities to their patients in these complex simulations and draw connections between communicating, involving a patient in their care, and promoting patient safety. Finally, we implore educators to measure behavior performance with other outcome variables to further investigate predictors of effective multitasking in multiple patients simulation.

Limitations

Sample characteristics represent a limitation to generalizability. We used convenience sampling from one university in one region of the country. As such, the sample may underrepresent some groups based on age, gender, race, ethnicity, and work experience. Findings from this study could be different if students from other universities were included in data collection. Despite this limitation, conclusions about temporal individual differences and how they impact multitasking behavior are novel to the nursing education literature.

Another limitation of this descriptive study relates to measurement of priority setting within the limited confines of a single scenario. Compared to computerized exercises capturing nurses’ priority setting and other higher-order thinking skills [53], conclusions about priority setting in this study are vulnerable to error. Results could differ if participants completed more multiple patients simulations with different populations over time.

Conclusion

This study of the relationships between temporal individual differences, demographic factors, cognitive workload, and multitasking adds to the literature because it describes predictors of nursing student success in multiple patients simulations. Standardized test scores appear to predict effective multitasking more than temporal individual differences such as DBTP. Nursing students who demonstrate multitasking behaviors tend to have a more balanced time perspective. Those feeling time pressure in multiple patients simulation may report feeling frustrated because they were not able to complete all of the tasks they envisioned, but evidence from the practice literature confirms our findings novice nurses feel time pressure during their first months of independent practice. Knowing students’ DBTP may help educators anticipate who will need more assistance with multitasking in simulation. Nursing students frequently wait until just before graduation to provide care for multiple patients; including mention of deviation from balanced time perspective in simulation preparation may help senior nursing students become more self-aware about and ultimately improve behavioral performance.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset used and analyzed during the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Change history

Notes

  1. It is difficult to parse out whether students’ behavior in simulation represents their true self or what they believe educators want to see/what educators are assessing. The bias of demand characteristics is a risk in all educational simulations and those meant for evaluation. Despite this potential confounder, simulation remains a more appropriate setting to assess nursing students’ multitasking than traditional clinical settings, especially because hospital-based educators repeatedly provide feedback that novice nurses’ problems with multitasking impact patient outcomes. Our descriptive design, without testable hypotheses, decreases the threat of demand bias interfering with findings. Participants and raters were not aware of the “answer” we wanted fulfilled in order to further protect against the bias of demand characteristics.

  2. The term meaningful was used based on the P value. Pre-2019, we would have said ATI scores were a “significant” predictor of CSEI scores. However, in April 2019, the American Statistical Association suggested formally the term “statistical significance” be abandoned along with the terms “statistically significant,” “p < 0.05,” and “nonsignificant.” Wasserstein and colleagues continue imploring researchers to remove these terms and similar terminology in favor of reporting exact p values and interpreting difference from a practical perspective [47].

Abbreviations

ATI:

Assessment Technologies Institute

BTP:

Balanced time perspective

CSEI:

Creighton Simulation Evaluation Instrument

DBTP:

Deviation from balanced time perspective

GPA:

Grade point average

NASA:

National Aeronautics and Space Administration

SD:

Standard deviation

TLX:

Task Load Index

References

  1. Spector N, Blegen M, Silvestre J, Barnsteiner J, Lynn MR, Ulrich B, et al. Transition to practice study in hospital settings. J Nurs Regul. 2015;5(4):24–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Massey D, Chaboyer W, Anderson V. What factors influence ward nurses’ recognition of and response to patient deterioration? An integrative review of the literature. Nurs Open. 2017; https://doi.org/10.1002/nop2.53.

  3. Hirani SAA, McFarlane J. Medication errors by novice nurses in a pediatric and neonatal care setting of Pakistan: Analysis of problems and proposed solutions. Pediatr Neonatal Nurs Open Access. 2016; https://doi.org/10.16966/2470-0983.112.

  4. Kiernan LC. Evaluating competence and confidence using simulation technology. Nursing. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NURSE.0000545022.36908.f3.

  5. Svenson O, Maule AJ. Time pressure and stress in human judgment and decision making. Boston: Springer; 1993.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  6. Rastegary H, Landy FJ. The interactions among time urgency, uncertainty, and time pressure. In: Svenson O, Maula AJ, editors. Time pressure and stress in human judgment and decision making. Boston: Springer; 1993. p. 217–39.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  7. Yen P-Y, Kellye M, Lopetegui M, Saha A, Loversidge J, Chipps EM, et al. Nurses’ time allocation and multitasking of nursing activities: A time motion study. AMIA. Annu Symp Proc AMIA Symp. 2018; https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30815156. Accessed 5 Dec 2018.

  8. Waller MJ, Franklin AE, Parcher DB. Time perspective balance and team adaptation in dynamic task contexts. J Organ Behav. https://doi.org/10.1002/job2431.

  9. Unison. Unsafe Staffing Levels Rising. 2015. Available from: https://www.unison.org.uk/content/uploads/2015/04/TowebRed-Alert-Unsafe-Staffing-Levels-Rising1.pdf. Accessed 14 April 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Yen PY, Kelley M, Lopetegui M, Rosado AL, Migliore EM, Chipps EM, et al. Understanding and visualizing multitasking and task switching activities: a time motion study to capture nursing workflow. AMIA. Annu Symp Proc AMIA Symp. 2016; https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5333222/. Accessed 22 Jan 2020.

  11. Bluedorn AC. The human organization of time: Temporal realities and experience. Stanford, CA: Stanford Business Books; 2002.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Conte JM, Landy FJ, Mathieu JE. Time urgency: conceptual and construct development. J Appl Psychol. 1995; https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.80.1.178.

  13. Conte JM, Rizzuto TE, Steiner DD. A construct-oriented analysis of individual-level polychronicity. J Manag. 1999; https://doi.org/10.1108/026839910263837.

  14. McLeod SA. Type A personality. Simply Psychology. 2017; https://www.simplypsychology.org/personality-a.html. Accessed.

  15. König CJ, Waller MJ. Time for reflection: A critical examination of polychronicity. Hum Perform. 2010; https://doi.org/10.1080/08959281003621703.

  16. Zimbardo PG, Boyd JN. Putting time in perspective: a valid, reliable individual-differences metric. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;77(6):1271–88.

  17. Waller MJ, Conte JM, Gibson CB, Carpenter MA. The effect of individual perceptions of deadlines on team performance. Acad Manag Rev. 2001; https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2001.5393894.

  18. Zimbardo ZM, McDermott R, Zimbardo PG. A new measure of the expanded present time perspective. In: Kostic A, editor. Time Perspective. London: Palgrave MacMillan; 2017. p. 41–62.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  19. Stolarski M, Wiberg B, Osin E. Assessing temporal harmony: the issue of a balanced time perspective. In: Time Perspective Theory; Review, Research and Application: Essays in Honor of Philip G Zimbardo; 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-07368-2_3.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  20. Stolarski M, Bitner J, Zimbardo PG. Time perspective, emotional intelligence and discounting of delayed awards. Time Soc. 2011; https://doi.org/10.1177/0961463X11414296.

  21. Zebardast A, Besharat MA, Hghighatgoo M. The relationship between self-regulation and time perspective in students. In: Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences; 2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.182.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Zhang JW, Howell RT, Stolarski M. Comparing three methods to measure a balanced time perspective: the relationship between a balanced time perspective and subjective well-being. J Happiness Stud. 2013; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9322-x.

  23. Mooney A, Earl JK, Mooney CH, Bateman H. Using balanced time perspective to explain well-being and planning in retirement. Front Psychol. 2017; https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01781.

  24. Jeffries PR, The NLN. Jeffries Simulation Theory. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Cook DA, Andersen DK, Combes JR, Feldman DL, Sachdeva AK. The value proposition of simulation-based education. Surg. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.surg.2017.11.008.

  26. Cook DA, Hatala R, Brydges R, Zendejas B, Szostek JH, Wang AT, et al. Technology-Enhanced Simulation for Health Professions Education: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. 2011;306(9):978–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cook DA, Brydges R, Hamstra SJ, Zendejas B, Szostek JH, Wang AT, et al. Comparative effectiveness of technology-enhanced simulation versus other instructional methods: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Simul Healthc. 2012; https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0b013e3182614f95.

  28. Weinger MB. The pharmacology of simulation: a conceptual framework to inform progress in simulation research. Simul Healthc. 2010; https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0b013e3181c91d4a.

  29. Cook DA. One drop at a time: Research to advance the science of simulation. Simul Healthc. 2010; https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0b013e3181c82aaa.

  30. Prince WL, Winmill D, Wing D, Kahoush A. Nursing students’ perceptions of a multiple-patient simulation experience. Nurs Educ Perspect. 2016; https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NEP.0000000000000046.

  31. World Medical Association. World medical association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Am Med Assoc. 2013; https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.281053.

  32. Poposki EM, Oswald FL. The multitasking preference inventory: toward an improved measure of individual differences in polychronicity. Hum Perform. 2010; https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2010.487843.

  33. Ballard DI, Seibold DR. Organizational members’ communication and temporal experience: Scale development and validation. Communic Res. 2004; https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650203261504.

  34. Landy FJ, Rastegary H, Thayer J, Colvin C. Time urgency: the construct and its measurement. J Appl Psychol. 1991; https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.76.5.644.

  35. Szameitat AJ, Hayati M. Gender differences in polychronicity. Front Psychol. 2019; https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30949103. Accessed.

  36. Hart SG. NASA-task load index (NASA-TLX); 20 years later. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc. 2006; https://doi.org/10.1177/154193120605000909.

  37. Rubio-Valdehita S, López-Higes R, Díaz-Ramiro E. Academic context and perceived mental workload of psychology students. Span J Psychol. 2014; https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2014.57.

  38. Franklin AE, Sideras S, Gubrud-Howe P, Lee CS. Comparison of expert modeling versus voice-over powerpoint lecture and presimulation readings on novice nurses’ competence of providing care to multiple patients. J Nurs Educ. 2014; https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20141023-01.

  39. Todd M, Manz JA, Hawkins KS, Parsons ME, Hercinger M. The development of a quantitative evaluation tool for simulations in nursing education. Int J Nurs Educ Scholarsh. 2008; https://doi.org/10.2202/1548-923X.1705.

  40. Parsons ME, Hawkins KS, Hercinger M, Todd M, Manz JA, Fang X. Improvement in Scoring Consistency for the Creighton Simulation Evaluation Instrument ©. Clin Simul Nurs. 2012; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecns.2012.02.008.

  41. Pearson K. On the criteria that a given system of deviations from the probable in the case of a system of correlated variables is such that is can be reasonably supposed to have arisen from random sampling. Philos Mag Ser. 1900;50(302):157–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Feinstein AR, Cicchetti DV. High agreement but low kappa: I. The problems of two paradoxes. J Clin Epidemiol. 1990;43(6):543–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kramer HC. Ramifications of a population model for observers might also complain about the reward k as a coefficient of reliability. Psychometrika. 1979;44:461–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Cicchetti DV, Feinstein AR. High agreement but low kappa: II. The problems of two paradoxes. J Clin Epidemiol. 1990;43(6):551–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gwet KL. Computing inter-rater reliability and its variance in the presence of high agreement. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2008; https://doi.org/10.1348/000711006X126600.

  46. Gwet KL. Handbook of inter-rater reliability: the definitive guide to measuring the extent of agreement among raters. 4th ed. Gaithersburg, MD: Advanced Analytics, LLC; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Wasserstein RL, Schirm AL, Lazar NA. Moving to a World Beyond “p < 0.05”. Am Stat. 2019; https://doi.org/10.1080/00031305.2019.1583913.

  48. Monagle JL, Lasater K, Stoyles S, Dieckmann N. New graduate nurse experiences in clinical judgment: What academic and practice educators need to know. Nurs Educ Perspect. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NEP.0000000000000336.

  49. Ebrahimi H, Hassankhani H, Negarandeh R, Azizi A, Gillespie M. Barriers to support for new graduated nurses in clinical settings: a qualitative study. Nurse Educ Today. 2016; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2015.11.008.

  50. Ironside PM, Jeffries PR, Martin A. Fostering patient safety competencies using multiple-patient simulation experiences. Nurs Outlook. 2009; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.outlook.2009.07.010.

  51. Kaplan B, Ura D. Use of multiple patient simulators to enhance prioritizing and delegating skills for senior nursing students. J Nurs Educ. 2010; https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20100331-07.

  52. Franklin AE, Sideras S, Dodd C, Hutson J. A randomized trial of multiple patient simulation preparation to improve novice nurses’ competence and self-efficacy. Nurs Educ Perspect. 2020; https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NEP.0000000000000593.

  53. Kavanagh JM, Szweda C. A crisis in competency: the strategic and ethical imperative to assessing new graduate nurses’ clinical reasoning. Nurs Educ Perspect. 2017; https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NEP.0000000000000112.

  54. Kalish BJ, Aerbersold M. Interruptions and multitasking in nursing care. Jt Comm J Qual Saf. 2010; https://doi.org/10.1016/S1553-7250(10)36021-1.

  55. Gevers JMP, Rutte CG, Van Eerde W. Meeting deadlines in work groups: Implicit and explicit mechanisms. Appl Psychol. 2006; https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00228.x.

  56. Bluedorn AC, Jaussi KS. Leaders, followers, and time. Leadersh Q. 2008; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2008.09.006.

  57. König CJ, Oberacher L, Kleinmann M. Personal and situational determinants of multitasking at work. J Pers Psychol. 2010; https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000008.

  58. Ishizaka K, Marshall SP, Conte JM. Individual differences in attentional strategies in multitasking situations. Hum Perform. 2001; https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327043HUP1404_4.

  59. Franklin AE, Lee CS. Effectiveness of simulation for improvement in self-efficacy among novice nurses: A meta-analysis. J Nurs Educ. 2014; https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20141023-03.

  60. National League for Nursing. Debriefing across the curriculum: a living document for the National League for Nursing. 2015. http://www.nln.org/docs/default-source/about/nln-vision-series-(position-statements)/nln-vision-debriefing-across-the-curriculum.pdf?sfvrsn=0. Accessed.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Murray M, Sundin D, Cope V. New graduate nurses’ understanding and attitudes about patient safety upon transition to practice. J Clin Nurs. 2019; https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.14839.

  62. Forsberg H, Athlin AM, Schwarz UV. Nurses’ perceptions of multitasking in the Emergency Department: effective, fun, and unproblematic (at least for me)—a qualitative study. Int Emerg Nurs. 2015; https://doi.org/10.1016/jienj.2014.05.002.

  63. Stolarski M, Zajenkowski M, Jankowski KS, Szymaniak K. Deviation from the balanced time perspective: a systematic review of empirical relationships with psychological variables. Pers Individ Dif. 2020; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109772.

  64. Brothers A, Gabrian M, Wahl HW, Diehl M. Future time perspective and awareness of age-related change: Examining their role in predicting psychological well-being. Psychol Aging. 2016;31(6):605–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Missen K, McKenna L, Beauchamp A. Work readiness of nursing graduates: Current perspectives of graduate nurse program coordinators. Contemp Nurse. 2015; https://doi.org/10.1080/10376178.2015.1095054.

  66. Beroz S. Outcome-based evaluation tool to evaluate student performance in high-fidelity simulation. Nurs Educ Perspect. 2016; https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NEP.0000000000000045.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge data collection assistance from Elaina Allen, Briana Jolley, and Beth Rogers, MSN, RN. Further, the authors thank their editor and anonymous reviewers for providing helpful comments and suggestions.

Funding

This project was not funded.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AF and MW designed the study, AF and LT collected data, and GG performed analysis. All authors interpreted data, contributed to, and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ashley E. Franklin.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We obtained ethics approval, and participants provided written informed consent. The Institutional Review Board at Texas Christian University approved this project (ref. 1806-109-1807).

Consent for publication

All authors approved the final manuscript and consent to publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Franklin, A.E., Thielke, L., Gilbert, G.E. et al. TIDES: examining the influence of temporal individual differences on multitasking in educational simulation. Adv Simul 5, 31 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41077-020-00144-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41077-020-00144-y

Keywords